This vignette contains answers to questions received from users or posted on discussion boards like Cross Validated and Stack Overflow
Inf
for the degrees of freedom?emmeans()
gives me pooled t tests, but I expected Welch's tEstimated marginal means (EMMs), a.k.a. least-squares means, are predictions on a reference grid of predictor settings, or marginal averages thereof. See details in the “basics” vignette.
There are two answers to this (i.e., be careful what you wish for):
emmeans(model, pairwise ~ treatment)
. This is the fastest way; however,
the results have a good chance of being invalid.The point here is that emmeans()
summarizes the model, not the data directly.
If you use a bad model, you will get bad results. And if you use a good model,
you will get appropriate results. It's up to you: it's your research—is it important?
Perhaps your question has to do with interacting factors, and you want to do some kind of post hoc analysis comparing levels of one (or more) of the factors on the response. Some specific versions of this question…
My first answer is: plots almost always help. If you have factors A, B, and C,
try something like emmip(model, A ~ B | C)
, which creates an interaction-style
plot of the predictions against B, for each A, with separate panels for each C.
This will help visualize what effects stand out in a practical way. This
can guide you in what post-hoc tests would make sense. See the
“interactions” vignette for more discussion and examples.
This is a situation where it may well be appropriate to compare the
slopes of trend lines, rather than the EMMs. See the
help("emtrends"()
“) and the discussion of this
topic in the "interactions” vignette
You need to be careful to define the reference grid consistently. For example,
if you use covariates x
and xsq
(equal to x^2
) to fit a quadratic curve,
the default reference grid uses the mean of each covariate – and mean(xsq)
is
usually not the same as mean(x)^2
. So you need to use at
to ensure that the
covariates are set consistently with respect to the model. See this subsection
of the “basics” vignette for an example.
That can happen because
it is just plain wrong to use [non-]overlapping CIs for individual means to do comparisons.
Look at the printed results from something like emmeans(mymodel, pairwise ~
treatment)
. In particular, note that the SE
values are not the same*, and
may even have different degrees of freedom. Means are one thing statistically,
and differences of means are quite another thing. Don't ever mix them up, and
don't ever use a CI display for comparing means.
I'll add that making hard-line decisions about “significant” and “non-significant” is in itself a poor practice. See the discussion in the “basics” vignette
This will happen if you fitted a model where the treatments you want to
compare were put in as a numeric predictor; for example dose
, with
values of 1, 2, and 3. If dose
is modeled as numeric, you will be
fitting a linear trend in those dose values, rather than a model
that allows those doses to differ in arbitrary ways. Go back and fit
a different model using factor(dose)
instead; it will make all the
difference. This is closely related to the next topic.
Equivalently, users ask how to get post hoc comparisons when we have covariates rather than factors. Yes, it does work, but you have to tell it the appropriate reference grid.
But before saying more, I have a question for you: Are you sure your model is meaningful?
sex
(coded 1 for female, 2 for male), no problem. The model will produce
the same predictions as you'd get if you'd used these as factors.brand
(coded 1 for Acme, 2 for Ajax, and
3 for Al's), this model is implying that the difference between
Acme and Ajax is exactly equal to the difference between Ajax and Al's,
owing to the fact that a linear trend in brand
has been fitted.
If you had instead coded 1 for Ajax, 2 for Al's, and 3 for Acme, the model
would produce different fitted values. You need to fit another model using
factor(brand)
in place of brand
.Assuming that issue is settled, you can do something like
emmeans(model, "sex", at = list(sex = 1:2))
, to get get separate
EMMs for each sex rather than one EMM for the average numeric value of sex
.
An alternative to the at
list is to use cov.reduce = FALSE
, which specifies
that the unique values of covariates are to be used rather than reducing them
to their means. However, the specification applies to all covariates, so
if you have another one, say age
, that has 43 different values in your data,
you will have a mess on your hands.
See “altering the reference grid” in the “basics” vignette for more discussion.
The emmeans package uses tookls in the estimability package to determine whether its results are uniquely estimable. For example, in a two-way model with interactions included, if there are no observations in a particular cell (factor combination), then we cannot estimate the mean of that cell.
When some of the EMMs are estimable and others are not, that is information about missing information in the data. If it's possible to remove some terms from the model (particularly interactions), that may make more things estimable if you re-fit with those terms excluded; but don't delete terms that are really needed for the model to fit well.
When all of the estimates are non-estimable, it could be symptomatic of something else. Some possibilities include:
nesting
argument. Perhaps the levels that B
can have depend on which level of A is in force. Then B is nested in A and
the model should specify A + A:B
, with no main effect for B
.state_name
as well
as dem_gov
which is coded 1 if the governor is a Democrat and 0 otherwise.
If the model was fitted with state_name
as a factor or character variable,
but dem_gov
as a numeric predictor, then, chances are, emmeans()
will
return non-estimable results. If instead, you use factor(dem_gov)
in the
model, then the fact that state_name
is nested in dem_gov
will be
detected, causing EMMs to be computed separately for each party's
states, thus making things estimable.pg <- transform(pigs, x = rep(1:3, c(10, 10, 9)))
pg.lm <- lm(log(conc) ~ x + source + factor(percent), data = pg)
emmeans(pg.lm, consec ~ percent)
## $emmeans
## percent emmean SE df asymp.LCL asymp.UCL
## 9 nonEst NA NA NA NA
## 12 nonEst NA NA NA NA
## 15 nonEst NA NA NA NA
## 18 nonEst NA NA NA NA
##
## Results are averaged over the levels of: source
## Results are given on the log (not the response) scale.
## Confidence level used: 0.95
##
## $contrasts
## contrast estimate SE df t.ratio p.value
## 12 - 9 0.1796 0.0561 23 3.202 0.0114
## 15 - 12 0.0378 0.0582 23 0.650 0.8613
## 18 - 15 0.0825 0.0691 23 1.194 0.5200
##
## Results are averaged over the levels of: source
## Results are given on the log (not the response) scale.
## P value adjustment: mvt method for 3 tests
The “messy-data” vignette has more examples and discussion.
Estimated marginal means summarize the model that you fitted to the data
– not the data themselves. Many of the most common models rely on
several simplifying assumptions – that certain effects are linear, that the
error variance is constant, etc. – and those assumptions are passed forward
into the emmeans()
results. Doing separate analyses on subsets usually
comprises departing from that overall model, so of course the results are
different.
First step: Carefully read the annotations below the output. Do they say
something like “results are on the log scale, not the response scale”?
If so, that explains it. A Poisson or logistic model involves a link function,
and by default, emmeans()
produces its results on that same scale.
You can add type = "response"
to the emmeans()
call and it will
put the results of the scale you expect. But that is not always
the best approach. The “transformations” vignette
has examples and discussion.
Inf
for the degrees of freedom? {#asymp}This is simply the way that emmeans labels asymptotic results (that is, estimates that are tested against the standard normal distribution – z tests – rather than the t distribution). Note that obtaining quantiles or probabilities from the t distribution with infinite degrees of freedom is the same as obtaining the corresponding values from the standard normal. For example:
qt(c(.9, .95, .975), df = Inf)
## [1] 1.281552 1.644854 1.959964
qnorm(c(.9, .95, .975))
## [1] 1.281552 1.644854 1.959964
so when you see infinite d.f., that just means its a z test or a z confidence interval.
As mentioned elsewhere, EMMs summarize a model, not the data.
If your model does not include any interactions between the by
variables
and the factors for which you want EMMs, then by definition, the
effects for the latter will be exactly the same regardless of the by
variable settings. So of course the comparisons will all be the same.
If you think they should be different, then you are saying that your model
should include interactions between the factors of interest and the
by
factors.
First of all, you should not be making binary decisions of “significant” or “nonsignificant.” This is a simplistic view of P values that assigns an unmerited magical quality to the value 0.05. It is suggested that you just report the P values actually obtained, and let your readers decide how significant your findings are in the context of the scientific findings.
But to answer the question: This is a common misunderstanding of ANOVA. If F has a particular P value, this implies only that some contrast among the means (or effects) has the same P value, after applying the Scheffe adjustment. That contrast may be very much unlike a pairwise comparison, especially when there are several means being compared. Having an F statistic with a P value of, say, 0.06, does not imply that any pairwise comparison will have a P value of 0.06 or smaller. Again referring to the paragraph above, just report the P value for each pairwise comparison, and don't try to relate them to the F statistic.
Another consideration is that by default, P values for pairwise comparisons are adjusted using the Tukey method, and the adjusted P values can be quite a bit larger than the unadjusted ones. (But I definitely do not advocate using no adjustment to “repair” this problem.)
There are two reasons this could happen:
by
group). Thus there is
only one comparison. When there is only one thing to test, there is no
multiplicity issue, and hence no multiplicity adjustment to the P values."mvt"
adjustment (which is exact); we don't default to
this because it can require a lot of computing time for a large set of
contrasts or comparisons.emmeans()
gives me pooled t tests, but I expected Welch's t {#nowelch}It is important to note that emmeans()
and its relatives produce results based
on the model object that you provide – not the data. So if your sample SDs
are wildly different, a model fitted using lm()
or aov()
is not a good model,
because those R functions use a statistical model that presumes that the errors
have constant variance. That is, the problem isn't in emmeans()
, it's in
handing it an inadequate model object.
Here is a simple illustrative example. Consider a simple one-way experiment and the following model:
mod1 <- aov(response ~ treat, data = mydata)
emmeans(mod1, pairwise ~ treat)
This code will estimate means and comparisons among treatments. All standard errors,
confidence intervals, and t statistics are based on the pooled residual SD
with N - k degrees of freedom (assuming N observations and k treatments).
These results are useful only if the underlying assumptions of mod1
are correct –
including the assumption that the error SD is the same for all treatments.
Alternatively, you could fit the following model using generalized least-squares:
mod2 = nlme::gls(response ~ treat, data = mydata,
weights = varIdent(form = ~1 | treat))
emmeans(mod2, pairwise ~ treat)
This model specifies that the error variance depends on the levels of treat
.
This would be a much better model to use when you have wildly different
sample SDs. The results of the emmeans()
call will reflect this improvement
in the modeling. The standard errors of the EMMs will depend on the individual
sample variances, and the t tests of the comparisons will be in essence
the Welch t statistics with Satterthwaite degrees of freedom.
To obtain appropriate post hoc estimates, contrasts, and comparisons,
one must first find a model that successfully explains the peculiarities in
the data. This point cannot be emphasized enough. If you give emmeans()
a
good model, you will obtain correct results; if you give it a bad model,
you will obtain incorrect results. Get the model right first.